From David Foster Wallace's Infinite Jest:
He could do the dextral pain the same way: Abiding. No one
single instant of it was unendurable. Here was a second right here: he
endured it. What was undealable-with was the thought of all the
instants all lined up and stretching ahead, glittering. And the
projected future fear... It's too much to think about. To Abide there.
But none of it's as of now real... He could just hunker down in the
space between each heartbeat and make each heartbeat a wall and live in
there. Not let his head look over. What's unendurable is what his own
head could make of it all. What his head could report to him, looking
over and ahead and reporting. But he could choose not to listen... He
hadn't quite gotten this before now, how it wasn't just the matter of
riding out cravings for a Substance: everything unendurable was in the
head, was the head not Abiding in the Present but hopping the wall and
doing a recon and then returning with unendurable news you then somehow
believed.
I've come to draw, or at to emphasize, a distinction separating two
realms between which I divide my time: real-land and head-land.
Real-land is the physical world, occupied by myself and billions of
equally real others, in which my fingers strike a series of keys and a
monitor displays strings of text corresponding to these keystrokes.
Head-land is the world in which I construct an image of what this
sentence will look like when complete, what this paragraph will look
like when complete and what this entire post will look like when
complete. And it doesn't stop there: in head-land, the finished post is
already being read, readers are reacting, readers are (or aren't)
responding and the resulting conversations are, for better or for
worse, playing themselves out. In head-land, the thoughts I've
translated into words and thus defined and developed in this post are
already shaping the thoughts to be explored in future posts, the
composition of which is going on there even now.
Head-land
is the setting of our predictions. When deciding what actions to take
in real-land, we don't base our choices on the possible actions' end
results in real-land — by definition, we don't know what will happen in
real-land until it already has — but on their end results in head-land.
The obvious problem: while real-land is built out of all the
information that exists everywhere, head-land is built not even out of
all the information in a single brain — and that isn't much — but of
whichever subset of that brain's information happens to be currently employed.
Hence the brutally rough, sometimes barely perceptible correspondence
between head-land and real-land in the short term and the nearly
nonexistent correspondence between them in the long.
One might
grant that while responding that running models in head-land is
nevertheless the best predictor of real-land events that any individual
has. And that's true, but it doesn't change our apparent tendency to
place far more trust in our head-land models than their dismal accuracy
could ever warrant. To take but one example: we really seem
to believe our own failures in head-land, head-land being the place we
do the vast majority — and in some cases, all — of our failing. How
many times has someone entertained the dream of, say, painting, but
then failed in head-land — couldn't get a head-land show, say, or
couldn't even mix head-land's colors right — and abandoned the
enterprise before beginning it? How many times has someone started
painting in real-land, gotten less-than-perfect results, and then
extrapolated that scrap of real-land data into a similarly crushing
head-land failure? Even established creators are vulnerable to this;
could the novelist suffering from a bout of "writer's block" simply be
the unwitting mark of a head-land vision of himself unable to write?
The danger of head-land catastrophes that poison real-land endeavors
looms over every step of the path. The possibility of being
metaphorically laughed out of the classroom, though probably only
illusory to begin with, never quite leaves one's mind. The same, to a
lesser extent, goes for experiencing rather than creating; someone who
refuses to listen to a new album, sample a new cuisine, watch a new
film or visit a new art exhibition on the excuse that they already
"know what [they] like" appear to have seen, and believed, their
head-land selves listening, eating or viewing with irritation,
repulsion or boredom.
That most of what we get worked up about
exists in our imaginations and our imaginations only is less a fresh
observation than the stuff of a thousand tired aphorisms. No battle
plan survives the first shot fired. You die a thousand deaths awaiting
the guillotine. Nothing's as good or as bad as you anticipate. You
never know until you try. We have nothing to fear but fear itself. Fear
is the mind-killer. It's all in your mind. Don't look down. Quoth John
Milton, "The mind is its own place, and in it self, can make a Heaven
of Hell, a Hell of Heaven." The more time I spend in head-land, the
less time I feel like I should spend in head-land, because
its awful, discouraging predictions are practically never borne out in
real-land. (Even when one comes close, head-land seems to fail to
capture the subjective experience of failure, which I find is either
never failure qua failure or always somehow psychologically
palliated or attenuated.) Experiencing a disaster in real-land is one
thing, and its negative effects are unavoidable, but experiencing
hypothetical head-land disasters as negative mental effects in real-land — which I suspect, we all do, and often — would seem to be optional.
Consider
global economic woes. While you more than likely know a few who've had
to take cuts in their incomes or find new ones, it's even likelier that
you're experiencing all the degradations of destitution in head-land
even as your real-land income has not and will not
substantially shrink. You're living out the agonies of fumbling with
food stamps in a long, angry grocery line despite the fact that you'll
never come close. When one is starved for real-land information, one's
head-land self gets hit with the worst possible fate. I hear of someone
dying in a gruesome real-land freak accident, and I die a dozen times
over in more gruesome, freakier head-land accidents. I visit a remote,
unpopulated real-land location and my head-land map contracts,
desolately and suffocatingly, to encompass only my lonely immediate
surroundings. (Then my glasses fall off and break. But there was time! There was time!)
I dream up an idea for implementation in real-land, but even before
I've fully articulated it my head-land self is already busy enduring
various ruinous executions of it. In head-land, worst-case scenarios
tend to become the scenarios, presenting huge, faultily-calculated sums of net present real-land misery.
Fear
of the ordeals that play out in head-land is a hindrance, but the
paralysis induced by the sheer weight of countless accumulated
hypothetical propositions is crippling. Even riding high on the hog in
real-land is no bulwark against the infinite (and infinitely bad)
vicissitudes of head-land. Say you're earning a pretty sweet living
playing the guitar in real-land. But what if you'd been born without
arms? Or in a time before the invention of the guitar? Or in a time
when you would've died of an infection before reaching age twelve? Then
you sure wouldn't be enjoying yourself as much. And what if you lose
your arms in a horrific fishing accident ten years down the line? Or if
you suddenly forget what a guitar is? Or if you die of an infection
anyway? Despite the fact that none of these dire possibilities have
occurred — or are even likely to occur — they're nonetheless inflicting
real-land pain from across the border.
I call this phenomenon a blooming what if
plant, beginning as the innocuous seed of a question — "What if I
hadn't done or encountered such and such earlier thing that proved to
be a necessary condition to something from which I enjoy and profit
now?" — and sprouting rapidly into a staggeringly complex organism, its
branches splitting into countless smaller branches which split into yet
more branches themselves. More perniciously, this also happens in a
situation-specific manner; namely, in situations whose sub-events are
particularly unpredictable. The classic example would be approaching
the girl one likes in middle school; the possible outcomes are so many
and varied, at least in the approacher's mind, that the what-ifs
multiply dizzyingly and collectively become unmanageable, especially if
his strategy is to prepare responses to all of them. It's no accident
that those never-get-the-girl mopes in movies spend so much time vainly
rehearsing conversations in advance, and that doing the same in life
never, ever works. There's a line to be drawn between the guys in
junior high who could talk the girls up effortlessly and the ones who
seized up merely contemplating it. I suspect the difference has to do
with the ratio of one's relative presence in head-land versus that in
real-land.
I would submit that, whatever their results, the
dudes who could walk right up to those girls and try their luck
habitually spent a lot more time in real-land than in head-land. They
probably weren't sitting around, eyes fixed on their own navels,
building elaborate fictions of inadequacy, embarrassment and ridicule;
if they were, wouldn't they have been just as paralyzed? They appeared
to operate on a mental model that either didn't conjure such dire
possibilities or, if it did, didn't allow them any decisionmaking
weight. "So the chick could turn me down? So what? What if space aliens
invade and destroy the Earth? I don't know what'll happen until I try."
This
brings up something else Wallace wrote and thought about — equivalent
verbs for him, I think — though not, as I dimly recall, in Infinite Jest.
In his sports journalism, of which he wrote some truly stunning pieces,
he kept looping back to the issue of the correlation and possible
causal connection between great athletes' brilliant physical
performance and their astonishing unreflectiveness in conversation and
prose. I'm thinking of Wallace's profile of Michael Joyce, a
not-quite-star tennis player who has no knowledge or interests outside
the game and couldn't even grasp the thundering sexual innuendo on a
billboard ad. I'm thinking of his review of Tracy Austin's
autobiography, a cardboard accretion of blithe assertions,
unreached-yet-strongly-stated conclusions and poster-grade sports
clichés. What must it be like, Wallace asked, to speak or hear prases
like "step it up" or "gotta concentrate now" and have them actually mean something? Is the sports star's nonexistent inner life not the price they pay for their astonishing athletic gift, but rather its very essence?
One can say many things about bigtime athletes, but that they live
in their heads is not one of them. I'd wager that you can't find a
group that spends less time in head-land than dedicated
athletes; they are, near-purely, creatures of real-land. The dudes who
could go right up to the ladies in seventh grade seemed to be, in kind
if not in magnitude, equally real-land's inhabitants. It comes as no
surprise that so many of them played sports and weren't often seen with
books. And not only were they undaunted by the danger (possibly because
unperceived) of crushing humiliation, I'd imagine they were inherently
less vulnerable to crushing humiliation in the first place, because
crushing humiliation, like theoretical arm loss and imagined endeavor
failure, is a head-land phenomenon. Humiliation is what makes a million
other-people-are-thinking-horrible-thoughts-about-me flowers bloom —
but only in head-land. The impact can only hit so hard if one
doesn't spend much time there, because in real-land, direct access to
another's thoughts is impossible. In head-land, one can't, as their
creator, help but have direct access to everyone else's thoughts, and thus if a head-land resident believes everyone's disparaging him, everyone is
disparaging him. "So what if they're thinking ill of me?" a full-time
real-land occupant might ask. "I can't know that for sure, and besides,
they're probably not; how often do you think, in a way that actually affects them, about someone who's been recently embarrassed?"
But there's a problem: saying someone "lives in their head" is more
or less synonymous with calling them intelligent. "Hey, look at that
brainy scientist go by, lost in thought; the fellow lives in his head!" As for professional athletes, well... let's just acknowledge the obvious, that professional athleticism is not
a byword for advanced intellectual capacity. (Wallace once lamented the
archetypal "basketball genius who cannot read".) So there's clearly a
return to time spent in head-land, and arguing for the benefits of
head-land occupancy even to nonintellectuals is a trivial task. How,
for instance, would we motivate ourselves without reference to
head-land? How could we envision possibilities and thus choose which
ones we'd like to realize without seeing them in head-land? Surely even
the most narrowly-focused, football-obsessed football player has
watched himself polish a Super Bowl ring in head-land. Why else would
he strive for that outcome? Head-land is where our fantasies happen,
where our goals are formulated, and is that a function we can do
without?
Hokey as it sounds, I do consider myself a somewhat "goal-oriented"
person, in that I burn a lot of time and mental bandwidth attempting to
realize certain head-land states. But, as the above paragraphs reveal,
I often experience head-land backfire in the form of discouraging
negative imaginings rather than encouraging positive ones. Here I could
simply pronounce that I will henceforth only use head-land for
envisioning the positive, but it's not quite that easy; I can think of
quite a few badly-ending head-land scenarios that I'm happy to
experience there — and only there — and take into account when making
real-land decisions. The head-land prediction that I'll get splattered
if I walk blindly into traffic comes to mind.
And I'm one of the less
head-land-bound people I know! I wouldn't be writing this post if I
didn't struggle with the damned place, but traits like my
near-inability to write fiction suggest that I don't gravitate toward
it as strongly as some. Still, I feel the need to minimize the problems
that spring forth from head-land without converting myself into an
impulsive dumb beast. The best compromise I have at the moment is not
necessarily to stem the flow of predictions out of head-land, but
simply to ignore the bulk of their content, to crank down
their resolution by 90% or so. Since the accuracy of our predictions
drops so precipitously as they extend forward in time and grow dense
with specifics, they'd mostly lose noise. Noise simply misleads, and
attenuating what misleads is the point of this exercise.
There
are countless practical ways to implement this. One quick-and-dirty
hack to dial down head-land's effect on your real-land calculations is
to only pay attention to head-land's shadow plays to the extent that
they're near your position in time. If they have to do with the distant
future, only consider their broadest outlines: the general nature of
the position you envision yourself occupying in twenty years, for
instance, rather than the specific event of your buxom assistant
bringing you just the right roast of coffee. If they have to do with
the past, near or distant, just chuck 'em; head-land models tend to run
wild with totally irrelevant oh-if-only-things-had-been-different
retrodictions, which are supremely tempting but ultimately
counterproductive. (As one incisive BEK cartoon had a therapist say,
"Woulda, shoulda, coulda — next!") If they have to do with the near
future, they're more valuable, and the nearer the future they deal
with, the better you would seem to do to pay attention to them.
The
concept behind this is one to which I've been devoting thought and
practice lately: small units of focus. Alas, this brings us to another
set of bromides, athletic and otherwise. One step at a time. Just you
and the goal. Break it down. Don't bite off more than you can chew. The
disturbing thing is how well operating on such a short horizon seems to
work, at least in certain contexts. I find I actually do run
better when I think only of the next yard, write better when I think
only of the next sentence and talk better when I think only of the
subject at hand. When my mind tries instead to load the entire run, the
entire essay or the entire conversation, head-land crashes. (This
applies to stuff traditionally thought of as more passive as well: I
read more effectively when I focus on the sentence, watch films more
effectively when I focus on the shot, listen to music more effectively
when I focus on the measure.) When Wallace writes about "the head not
Abiding in the Present but hopping the wall and doing a recon and then
returning with unendurable news" and "[hunkering] down in the space
between each heartbeat and [making] each heartbeat a wall and [living]
in there", I think this is what he means.
Ignoring all head-land
details past a certain threshold, de-weighting head-land predictions
with their distance in the future and focusing primarily on small,
discrete-seeming, temporally proximate units aren't just techniques to
evade internal discouragement, either; they also guard against the
perhaps even more sinister (and certainly sneakier) forces of
complacency. While failure in head-land can cause one to pack it in in
real-land, success in head-land, which is merely a daydream away, can prevent one from even trying in real-land. I can't put it better than Paul Graham
does: "If you have a day job you don't take seriously because you plan
to be a novelist, are you producing? Are you writing pages of fiction,
however bad? As long as you're producing, you'll know you're not merely
using the hazy vision of the grand novel you plan to write one day as
an opiate."
This is why I'm starting to believe that coming up
with great ideas in head-land and then executing them in real-land may
be a misconceived process, or at least suboptimally conceived one. How
many projects have been forever delayed because the creator decided to
wait until the "idea" was just a little bit better, or, in other words,
until the head-land simulation came out a little more favorably? It's
plausible that this type of stalling lies at the heart of
procrastination: one puts the job off until tomorrow because the
head-land model doesn't show it as turning out perfect today, never
mind the facts that (a) it'll never be perfect, no matter when it's started and (b)
it's unlikely to turn out better with less time available for the work,
especially given the unforeseen troubles and opportunities. I
provisionally believe that this a priori, head-land idea
stuff can be profitably be replaced with small-scale real-land
exploratory actions that demand little in the way of time or resource
investment. Rather than executing steps one through one hundred in
head-land, execute step one in real-land; if nothing else, the data you
get in response will be infinitely more reliable and more useful in
determining what step two should involve. Those dudes in middle school
knew this on some basic level: you just gotta go up to the girl and say
something. It's the only gauge you have of whether you should say
something more, and of what that something should be. It's all about
hammering in the thin end of the wedge.
For what it's worth,
I've found this borne out in what little creation I've done thus far.
I've reached the point of accepting that I don't know — can't
know — how a project's going to turn out, since each step depends on
the accumulated effects of the steps that preceded it. All I can do is
get clear on my vague, broad goal and put my best foot forward, keeping
my mind open to accept all relevant information as it develops. When I
started my first radio show, I had a bunch of head-land projections
about how the show would be, but in practice it evolved away from them
in real-land rather sharply — and, I think, for the better. When I
started another one a year later, I knew to factor in this
unforeseeable real-land evolution from the get-go and thus kept my
ideas about what it was supposed to me broad, flexible and small in
number, letting the events of real-land fill in the details as they
might. With a TV project only just started, I've tried my hardest to
stay out of head-land as much as possible; the bajillion variables
involved would send whatever old, buggy software my head-land modeler
uses straight to the Blue Screen of Death. (Yes, our brains are
Windows-based.) Even if it didn't crash, it's not as if I'd be getting
sterling predictions out of it. I have, more grandly speaking, come to
accept much more of the future's unknowability than once I did; that
goes double for the future of my own works. Modeling a successful work
in head-land now seems a badly flawed strategy, to be replaced by
taking small steps in real-land and working with its response.
I
could frame this as another rung in thet climb from a thought-heavier
life to an action-heavier life. of approaching and affecting the world as it exists in real-land
rather than as it is imagined in head-land. I've nevery been what one
would call an idealist and I suppose I'm drawing no closer to that
label. Some regard flight from idealism as flight toward cynicism, but
it's cynicism I'm been fleeing as well, perhaps even primarily; what is
cynicism, after all, but a mistaken reliance on pessimistic head-land
conclusions?